Search






Jeff's Amazon.com Wish List

Archive Calendar

November 2024
M T W T F S S
 123
45678910
11121314151617
18192021222324
252627282930  

Archives

From 2010: “Just who is responsible for the ‘unconscious’: meaning, intent, and the use of ‘false consciousness’ in the making of identity politics

At the risk of bringing up a sore subject… From NPR, “How ‘The Hidden Brain’ Does The Thinking For Us”:

After making a silly mistake, it’s not uncommon for a person to say, “Oops — I was on autopilot.” In his new book, The Hidden Brain, science writer Shankar Vedantam explains how there’s actually a lot of truth to that.

Our brains have two modes, he tells NPR’s Steve Inkseep — conscious and unconscious, pilot and autopilot — and we are constantly switching back and forth between the two.

First, let me interrupt here to point out the obvious: even if we wish to argue with Mr Vedantam’s formulation — that is, even were we to take issue with the reduction of the brain into binary modes — what is important to keep in mind here is that both of these modes, conscious and unconscious, belong to us and so are ours, a product of the agency that is, in fact, us. This seems like a simple enough observation, but as the political arguments stemming from such descriptions tend to develop, this truism often gets lost, or at least “bracketed” by those who wish to use the observation in incoherent (and yet politically powerful) ways, as we will see momentarily.

Onward:

“The problem arises when we [switch] without our awareness,” Vedantam says, “and the autopilot ends up flying the plane, when we should be flying the plane” [my emphasis]

— And there it is, the subtle switch, the moment at which what begins as a descriptive metaphor becomes a political tool that deviates from its own kernel assumptions. To wit: Notice that what Vedantam does here is maneuver from a binary brain mode — conscious and unconscious, both of which we have established belong to us — to something like two separate and unique brains, one that runs the autopilot, the other of which belongs to a “we” that is now divorced from the “autopilot” mode.

And if it isn’t we who are running the autopilot, Vedantam will wonder, how then to account for it?

The autopilot mode can be useful when we’re multitasking, but it can also lead us to make unsupported snap judgments about people in the world around us. Vedantam says that when we interact with people from different backgrounds in high-pressure situations, it’s easy to rely — unconsciously — on heuristics.

Racial categorization begins at an extremely early age. Vedantam cites research from a day-care center in Montreal that found that children as young as 3 linked white faces with positive attributes and black faces with negative attributes.

Now we’re off and running: a day-care center, in its own specific geographical and political context, will be allowed, for the sake of this argument, to stand in as representative of how ALL children learn racial differences — a dubious control group for a scientist to use, especially when drawing conclusions from what is a rather pedestrian hypothesis. But no worries: he can be forgiven, provided he reaches the right conclusions.

Of course, were the data reversed (had, for instance, the day-care center under review been located in the basement of Reverend Wright’s church, say) — with whites linked to negative attributes and blacks viewed positively — that data almost certainly wouldn’t be extrapolated out as normative the way it is here. In fact, such data would likely be used to exhort the force of identity politics to “empower” historically disenfranchised groups, the result being that we must now believe that identity politics is simultaneously ameliorative (when it empowers certain identity groups) and “racist” (when it empowers other identity groups), even as the mechanism is precisely the same.

“Now, these were children who are 3 years old,” Vedantam says. “It is especially hard to call them bigots, or to suggest that they are explicitly racially biased or have animosity in their hearts.”

Vedantam says the mind is hard-wired to “form associations between people and concepts.” But he thinks that the links the children made between particular groups and particular concepts were not biologically based — those judgments came from culture and upbringing.

He says that for every 50 times a year a teacher talks about tolerance, there are many hundreds of implicit messages of racial bias that children absorb through culture — whether it’s television, books or the attitudes of the adults and kids around them.

“And it’s these hidden associations that essentially determine what happens in the unconscious minds of these children,” Vedantam says.

And here you have the last two maneuvers: 1) It is silly to call children as young as 3 bigots, Vedantam will (pretend to) concede; and yet they are showing bigoted behavior — like, for instance, they draw “bigoted associations” or make “racist statements” — which transgressions Vedantam will trace to “culture and upbringing”. Are these children responsible for their own culture? Their own upbringing? Of course not, the argument will suggest. And so their bigotry, which is undeniable (given the “associations” drawn by the kids in one Montreal day-care center) must come from somewhere else, and must be lodged somewhere outside of the conscious reach of these children (where presumably it could be corrected).

Once we are here — once we begin to give power to deeply-seeded attitudes learned through acculturation and rote indoctrination (and buried deep in our “sub-conscious”) while simultaneously divorcing the conscious mind from the unconscious mind in such a way that the unconscious mind is no longer a part of the intentional “we” — it is an easy next step to argue 2) that “we” are not responsible for any kind of unconscious racism or bigotry; thus, we can say racist things, or make racist associations, without those associations or statements being intentionally racist. More, we can’t be expected to recognize in ourselves such unconscious bigotry precisely because it lies in our unconscious mind, which is the “autopilot” to our “we,” and as such stands apart from our conscious control over it. Which means we’ll have to rely on others to spot our bigotry for us. God bless ’em.

Can we therefore say something racist without being racist? Well, yes and no: “we” can be racist, but it is really “culture and society” that has programmed that racism into us. And so when we repeat those racist lessons taught us by culture and society, we are being racist — but the “we” in question is not so much us as it is the society and culture that inscribes (and so owns) our unconscious mind. It is the autopilot, the part of us that is no longer really us, separated as it is from the “we” that makes up our conscious mind. To deny it is to engage in “false consciousness” — and you are denying that part of yourself that others get to define. Which marks you as deluded and duped.

Or, to put it another way, yes, you are racist. But so is everyone else who was inscribed by the same culture and society as you. So don’t sweat it.

The upshot of all this is that we are left with an obvious way to fight “racism”: change society and culture in such a way that our “unconscious” mind — over which we have limited ownership (or rather, something akin to a rental agreement) — learns the “correct” lessons. We need to be taught which kinds of associations are acceptable and which are not. Our speech and thought needs to be cleansed; our autopilot re-educated.

And thankfully, plenty of folks from various identity groups will be happy to teach you the “non-bigoted” way you must think about them.

“We tend to think of the conscious messages that we give children as being the most powerful education that we can give them,” Vedantam says — but the unconscious messages are actually far more influential.

In American society, colorblindness is often held up as the ideal. And though it’s a worthy aspiration, Vedantam says it’s a goal that isn’t rooted in psychological reality.

“Our hidden brains will always recognize people’s races, and they will do so from a very, very young age,” Vedantam says. “The far better approach is to put race on the table, to ask [children] to unpack the associations that they are learning, to help us shape those associations in more effective ways.”

— And so we reach the end point.

Colorblindness sounds good, but because kids will always make associations (and one they will “always” make is based on “race”), our only choice is to teach them at a young age that much of what they learn from their own experiential encounters with the world is wrong, particularly when that experience rubs against some group-sanctioned narrative. And so we must teach children to stop believing themselves, and start believing only those with the “authenticity” to draw “accurate” and approved associations.

Of course, what Vedantam doesn’t say is that “race” is, itself, a learned category — one that differs from mere pigmentation — and so it would, presumably, be just as easy to “unpack” racialist arguments early on, which is precisely what the idea of “colorblindness” endeavors to do. More, Vedantam seems to believe that merely recognizing differences in pigmentation has some sort of causal relationship to bigotry — that the negative and positive associations attributed to different colors by those in some Montreal day-care center are the result of color alone as it is filtered through cultural markers and societal cues.

But just because culture and society leads one to make politically incorrect associations doesn’t mean they’ve made incorrect or unreasonable associations — ones that as they become more socially aware and more logically savvy they will be able to disentangle as either causal or not, as having merit or not.

Identity politics wants to “help” us through that learning process by taking us right to the conclusions we “should” be reaching. And to do so, it hopes to force on “culture” and “society” certain ways of thinking and talking that will shape the “proper” associations in children.

In short, it wants to brainwash them. But for their own good.

Because after all, a little bigoted autopilot is a dangerous thing…

Going back to the autopilot analogy, Vedantam says it’s not a problem that the brain has an autopilot mode — as long as you are aware of when it is on. His book, The Hidden Brain, is about how to “take back the controls.”

On offer here is the following prescription: you can only know your autopilot by learning what culture and society have imprinted upon you. Once there, you can only “take back control” by changing what culture and society imprint. Because otherwise, nothing else Vedantam writes makes sense: if you could consciously control your unconscious, that would be a form of consciousness that robs the unconscious of its (presumed) power; so the answer is that you must control your unconscious mind by consciously decided what is appropriate for it to learn in the first place.

Which is to say, you can only take back control by giving over control to those who will properly teach you.

So if the human psyche is just a big constellation of conscious and unconscious cognition — which thoughts represent the real you?

“Most of us think of ourselves as being conscious, intentional, deliberate creatures,” Vedantam says. “I know that I think of myself that way: I know why I like this movie star, or why I voted for this president, or why I prefer this political party to that.”

But doing research for this book changed all that, Vedantam says.

“I have become, in some ways, much more humble about my views and much less certain about myself. And it may well be that the hidden brain is much more in charge of what we do than our conscious mind’s intentions.”

If the “hidden brain” were really just our unconscious, Vedantam’s conclusion would read something like this: “It may well be that we are much more in charge of what we do than, well, we are.”

— Which doesn’t sound quite so thoughtful — and has the extra added problem of pointing out that we are, in fact, responsible for ourselves, if only because it is US who act as the filter between culture and society and what we become.

“You” are the sum of your agency, which includes both your unconscious and conscious minds. In fact, the two are inseparable as components of your agency.

And while it may be easy to blame society for bigotries that are yours, you own them, whether you are 3 or 33. Just because society tells you to jump off a cliff…

Once you recognize that, you can turn your attention to the proper question: what is it that makes some associations de facto bigoted in the first place? And who has the right to declare them such…?

Trace the answer back to the source, and it’s easy to see why some much time and energy is invested into dividing you from you.

h/t Terry H.

****
update: my response to Helian Unbound, here.

18 Replies to “From 2010: “Just who is responsible for the ‘unconscious’: meaning, intent, and the use of ‘false consciousness’ in the making of identity politics”

  1. happyfeet says:

    i just moved and i’m worried to ask miguel to change the side the fridge opens on cause of it might could be white privilege, so i have to ask my transcultural friend F for an advise

  2. JHoward says:

    …the subtle switch, the moment at which what begins as a descriptive metaphor becomes a political tool that deviates from its own kernel assumptions.

    There are a number of reasons to find this is a conscious maneuver, meaning a lie, one aimed at gaining what amounts to an eventual political advantage. Since politics are downstream of culture, and since politics are in essence the public manifestation of joint personal morality, just carried off in such a way to further the, er, general welfare, then the lie is confirmed: You lie when you mean to be functionally misunderstood. To slide a big fat one in over the transom.

    This then is the malcontent’s role and aim. His or her intent is deception.

    How did we arrive at “malcontent”? By admission these people are progressing from one approximate point to another, the first being defined by their target’s inherent, incumbent flaws – those of the conservably meritorious kind that must be progressed from – and the second being open for analysis once they’re reached, like finding out what’s in the bill after it’s passed.

    The fact this constutites the force of the tyranny of the mob is irrelevant to the progressing warrior. The fact that this calls for lying – for deception, intent-robbing, et al – is likewise a sacrifice s/he’s willing to make, not unlike Alinsky’s lofty goals or Marx’s collective goodness or various murderous People’s Revolutions; the goal is always worth the costs; the ends always justify the means.

    Meanwhile the ostensible right, the entity that contains the individual instances of its own structural, public manifestation of individual ethic and merit, they being the abolition of force, has as its endless task just that: To identify when this force occurs and to thwart the mob that wants to impose it. For there is nothing about leftism that is not first a presumption of goodness carried on a wave of earnest force.

    The true progression is that of the lie being crammed into popular acceptability, the intent of the normal structuralist and his normal ethics, rhetoric, and methods being crafted into something they’re not, and the whole mess constituting an acceptable new force in society where the perpetrator becomes sanctified by popular assumption and the victim becomes the condemned by popular acclaim.

    And this is why every time the ostensible right adopts any of this progression on any of its levels – they all being either dishonesty or force – the right becomes part of the problem, right down to arguing any of it on merits, for it has none.

    Until normal people learn not to be codependent they’ll be codependent, which in this instance is why the right is and has lost. It and they cannot see that they’ve been robbed of their perfectly normal credibility – that and the structures they should have long identified as rationally principled instead of the soft collectivist crap they have already co-opted as structural – so they will continue to mislead themselves.

    The normie rightist is misguided when s/he starts by making a subtle rhetorical mistake: When he attempts to use progressivism’s inherent hypocrisy as a counter-argument pro-reason but ends up slowly adopting progressive causes as his own just out of sheer defensive opposition. The slow shift in the polarity and populism of racialism is a good example of what happens when a projected personal value becomes a popular public sentiment out of simple defensive relocation. And since the progressive cause is neither structural or original or therefore inherently principled – since racism is a personal moral matter in some matrix of other moral elements – the normie rightist has, in effect, begun to subjectively remodel the collective virtue, which then leans on just another version of an ersatz collective morality and eventually competes to become typically shoddy public policy.

    In other words, to become force.

    Oddly, the easiest argument to make is the one in favor of classical liberalism, which in essence states that anything public shall not be based on arbitrary, popular force but instead on the negative right, of which there are just a few, and on the structural, honest individual … of which there are just a few.

    But by now the list of public forces owned by the normie right is as long and onerous as that of his opponent. This is what is meant by repeating the lie long enough that it becomes a truth. And the first lie in this case is that of the theft of meaning.

  3. happyfeet says:

    i had to do it myself cause of social justice

  4. newrouter says:

    Methinks 5/8″ nut driver and torix #15 or #20 for refrigerator doors (whiripool).

  5. Squid says:

    Am I the only one who sees this argument as just another manifestation of the left’s Original Sin gambit?

    “Yes, you’re a racist, but it’s not your fault — your racism was programmed into you since infancy. But if you join the One True Faith and repent your racist sins*, you can be redeemed**!”

    * of course, true repentance comes from voting correctly and donating time and money to approved causes and candidates.

    ** by “redeemed,” we mean “invited to the cool parties.”

  6. Were the day care kids white or black? Asian or “First Nations”. Francophone or English only? I bet the results speak for themselves.

    At any rate, babies can tell strangers pretty damn early, and kids put in to stressful situations will be a bit more squirrelly than kids that aren’t. These kids are probably seeing “different” as “danger”. It’s built in. We are, after all, still animals. We used to understand this about people, the fact that we don’t now scares me. Remember, it took us 1300 years to re-invent hydraulic cement.

  7. newrouter says:

    >“Yes, you’re a racist, but it’s not your fault — your racism was programmed into you since infancy.“Yes, you’re “low IQ”, but it’s not your fault — your “low IQ” was programmed into you since infancy.

  8. dicentra says:

    The “conscious/unconscious” dichotomy that he poses is akin to the “fallen man/enlightened man” paradigm found in many religions, but it suffers from the assumption that the “unconscious” is programmed by culture.

    It’s more useful to recognize the three main regions of the brain: the lizard brain, the mammal brain, and the frontal lobes. The lizard brain is the most primitive, taking care of breathing and digestion and other autonomic functions. The mammal brain has our passions and desires and emotions, and the frontal lobes have reasoning, conscience, and decision-making. Plenty of interaction among the three.

    The mammal brain is where we get our instinctive fear of strangers (whom we know by their appearance) and our desire to live in tribes (us against them). We inherit this from our mammalian ancestors. We do learn from experience who constitutes “us” and who’s the stranger. But the suspicion of strangers is inborn.

    Our frontal lobes give us the ability to recognize this instinct and to override it through reason: yes, that family who moved in next door has a different complexion than I have (or they speak a different language), but reason tells me that they’re not a threat, and religion tells me they’re my brothers and sisters, so the moral thing to do is reject the instinctive suspicion and embrace them as “us.”

    It’s not a matter, therefore, of refactoring society to refactor the mammal brain, because it can’t be done. We’re stuck with our mammal instincts. The best we can do is recognize that the instincts are there and through the use of reason behave in a more enlightened manner.

    Persuasion, in other words. Convince people that the world is a better place if that mammalian instinct is overridden by reason. Help people see other people as “us” rather than “them.”

    The woke project, however, does its utmost to emphasize differences, laying down linguistic land mines so that we HAVE TO be suspicious of other people, because we’ve learned that the slightest mistake on our part will result in Bad Things.

    In that case, it’s best to stick with your “own kind” to avoid the land mines. Best not trigger anyone. Best withdraw from society lest you be digitally lynched.

    Bastards know they’re not actually helping people overcome their natural prejudices. They know they’re stirring up strife and conflict. They thrive on it.

    Chaos favors the tyrant.

  9. guinspen says:

    Bingo, Dice.

    Still, being over the hill, all that’s left for me is to twist and shout in the wind.

    So to speak.

  10. palaeomerus says:

    I’m just happy that my autocomplete takes me here when I type Pr again.

    Maybe Burger Chef will come back too.

    Missing the old world a little.

  11. JHoward says:

    When both sides of the divide are fundamentally wrong:

    https://thehill.com/opinion/finance/452045-make-americas-deficits-great-again

  12. happyfeet says:

    seems like we’re in a lull

  13. happyfeet says:

    reattaching the proposal for your convenience (attached)

    let’s circle back on this soon and talk next steps

  14. guinspen says:

    Respectfully, go shoeshine yourself.

  15. happyfeet says:

    you respectfully ha!

    it’s summertime so i got up early and did a picture of the dollyparton on broadway

  16. guinspen says:

    Twit does tits, woot!

  17. happyfeet says:

    NO you have it all wrong

  18. happyfeet says:

    http://www.edgevillebuzz.com/arts-2/hello-dolly-edgewater-duo-turns-graffiti-eyesore-pop-art-sensation

    The Lytles say Dolly is an important female icon as well, as she has came from nothing and slowly became an incredibly important female business force. The fact that the image does not include the body parts that helped make Dolly famous (her breasts) has caused many not to recognize the woman behind the image. Michelle believes her artwork showcases why many people truly love Dolly, for her talent and her hard work.

Comments are closed.